Find information on thousands of medical conditions and prescription drugs.

Sudden infant death syndrome

Sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) is any sudden and unexplained death of an apparently healthy infant aged one month to one year. The term cot death is sometimes used in the United Kingdom, and crib death in the United States. more...

Home
Diseases
A
B
C
D
E
F
G
H
I
J
K
L
M
N
O
P
Q
R
S
Sabinas brittle hair...
Saccharopinuria
Sacral agenesis
Saethre-Chotzen syndrome
Salla disease
Salmonellosis
Sandhoff disease
Sanfilippo syndrome
Sarcoidosis
Say Meyer syndrome
Scabies
Scabiophobia
Scarlet fever
Schamberg disease...
Schistosomiasis
Schizencephaly
Schizophrenia
Schmitt Gillenwater Kelly...
Sciatica
Scimitar syndrome
Sciophobia
Scleroderma
Scrapie
Scurvy
Selachophobia
Selective mutism
Seminoma
Sensorineural hearing loss
Seplophobia
Sepsis
Septo-optic dysplasia
Serum sickness
Severe acute respiratory...
Severe combined...
Sezary syndrome
Sheehan syndrome
Shigellosis
Shingles
Shock
Short bowel syndrome
Short QT syndrome
Shprintzen syndrome
Shulman-Upshaw syndrome
Shwachman syndrome
Shwachman-Diamond syndrome
Shy-Drager syndrome
Sialidosis
Sickle-cell disease
Sickle-cell disease
Sickle-cell disease
Siderosis
Silicosis
Silver-Russell dwarfism
Sipple syndrome
Sirenomelia
Sjogren's syndrome
Sly syndrome
Smallpox
Smith-Magenis Syndrome
Sociophobia
Soft tissue sarcoma
Somniphobia
Sotos syndrome
Spasmodic dysphonia
Spasmodic torticollis
Spherocytosis
Sphingolipidosis
Spinal cord injury
Spinal muscular atrophy
Spinal shock
Spinal stenosis
Spinocerebellar ataxia
Splenic-flexure syndrome
Splenomegaly
Spondylitis
Spondyloepiphyseal...
Spondylometaphyseal...
Sporotrichosis
Squamous cell carcinoma
St. Anthony's fire
Stein-Leventhal syndrome
Stevens-Johnson syndrome
Stickler syndrome
Stiff man syndrome
Still's disease
Stomach cancer
Stomatitis
Strabismus
Strep throat
Strongyloidiasis
Strumpell-lorrain disease
Sturge-Weber syndrome
Subacute sclerosing...
Sudden infant death syndrome
Sugarman syndrome
Sweet syndrome
Swimmer's ear
Swyer syndrome
Sydenham's chorea
Syncope
Syndactyly
Syndrome X
Synovial osteochondromatosis
Synovial sarcoma
Synovitis
Syphilis
Syringomas
Syringomyelia
Systemic carnitine...
Systemic lupus erythematosus
Systemic mastocytosis
Systemic sclerosis
T
U
V
W
X
Y
Z
Medicines

Diagnosis

SIDS is a definition of exclusion and only applies to an infant whose death remains unexplained after the performance of an adequate postmortem investigation including (1) an autopsy, (2) investigation of the scene and circumstances of the death and (3) exploration of the medical history of the infant and family. Generally, but not always, the infant is found dead after having been put to sleep and exhibits no signs of having suffered.

The inexplicability of the death often leaves parents with a deep sense of guilt in addition to their grief.

Statistics

SIDS is responsible for roughly 50 deaths per 100,000 births in the US. It is responsible for far fewer deaths than congenital disorders and disorders related to short gestation; though it becomes the leading cause of death in otherwise healthy babies after one month of age.

The frequency of SIDS appears to be a strong function of the age, race, education, and socio-economic status of the parents.

Risk factors

Very little is known about the possible causes of SIDS; there is no method for absolute prevention. However, several risk factors are associated with increased probability of the syndrome.

Prenatal risks

  • inadequate prenatal care
  • inadequate prenatal nutrition
  • tobacco smoking
  • use of cocaine or heroin
  • teenage pregnancy
  • less than a one year interval between subsequent births

Post-natal risks

  • low birth weight (especially less than 1.5 kg)
  • exposure to tobacco smoke
  • laying an infant to sleep on his or her stomach (see positional plagiocephaly)
  • failure to breastfeed
  • excess clothing and overheating
  • excess bedding, soft sleep surface and stuffed animals
  • sex (60% of deaths occur in males)
  • age (incidence is higher between 2-4 months)

In addition, research indicates a reduced risk of SIDS in conjunction with a safe co-sleeping arrangement. Though findings are still preliminary, the proximity of a parent's respiration is thought to stimulate proper respiratory development in the infant.

(The use of baby monitors, particularly those with motion sensors, can allow the parents to remotely keep track of their child.)

SIDS and child abuse

Controversial British pediatrician Roy Meadow believes that many cases diagnosed as SIDS are really the result of child abuse on the part of a parent suffering from Munchausen Syndrome by Proxy (a condition which he himself identified). During the 1990s and early 2000s, a great many mothers of multiple apparent SIDS victims were convicted of murder on the basis of Meadow's opinion. However, in 2003 a number of high-profile acquittals brought Sir Roy's theories into disrepute, and many now doubt their credibility. Several hundred murder convictions are now under review.

Read more at Wikipedia.org


[List your site here Free!]


Babies sleeping with parents: case-control study of factors influencing the risk of the sudden infant death syndrome
From British Medical Journal, 12/4/99 by Peter S Blair

Abstract

Objective To investigate the risks of the sudden infant death syndrome and factors that may contribute to unsafe sleeping environments.

Design Three year, population based case-control study. Parental interviews were conducted for each sudden infant death and for four controls matched for age, locality, and time of sleep.

Setting Five regions in England with a total population of over 17 million people.

Subjects 325 babies who died and 1300 control infants.

Results In the multivariate analysis infants who shared their parents' bed and were then put back in their own cot had no increased risk (odds ratio 0.67; 95% confidence interval 0.22 to 2.00). There was an increased risk for infants who shared the bed for the whole sleep or were taken to and found in the parental bed (9.78; 4.02 to 23.83), infants who slept in a separate room from their parents (10.49; 4.26 to 25.81), and infants who shared a sofa (48.99; 5.04 to 475.60). The risk associated with being found in the parental bed was not significant for older infants ([is greater than] 14 weeks) or for infants of parents who did not smoke and became non-significant after adjustment for recent maternal alcohol consumption ([is greater than] 2 units), use of duvets ([is greater than] 4 togs), parental tiredness (infant slept [is less than or equal to] 4 hours for longest sleep in previous 24 hours), and overcrowded housing conditions ([is greater than] 2 people per room of the house).

Conclusions There are certain circumstances when bed sharing should be avoided, particularly for infants under four months old. Parents sleeping on a sofa with infants should always be avoided. There is no evidence that bed sharing is hazardous for infants of parents who do not smoke.

Introduction

In most non-westernised cultures the mother commonly shares a bed with her infant.[1] Postulated physiological benefits of close contact between infants and care givers include improved cardiorespiratory stability and oxygenation, fewer episodes of crying, better thermoregulation, an increased prevalence and duration of breast feeding, and enhanced milk production.[2 3]

Before the reduction in the rate of the sudden infant death syndrome there was conflicting evidence on the effect of bed sharing.[4-6] Early observational studies suggested parental alcohol consumption, drug ingestion, obesity, and fatigue to support the concept that parents may lie on an infant who shares the bed with them.[7-9] Data from New Zealand implicated bed sharing as a risk factor for sudden infant death.[10]

The importance of socioeconomic deprivation and bed sharing has been highlighted in New Zealand among the Maori population,[11] which has high rates of smoking and alcohol consumption, and in the United States among poor black populations.[12] In certain Asian cultures, however, where particular forms of mother-infant cosleeping are common such as in Japan[13] and Hong Kong,[14] the rates of cot death are low, corresponding to findings in the Bangladeshi[15] and Asian[16] communities in the United Kingdom and the Pacific Island communities in New Zealand.[17]

While the benefits of the supine sleeping position for infants are now clear, there is no consensus on where the infant should sleep in relation to the parents. The study of sudden unexpected deaths in infancy (part of the confidential inquiry into stillbirths and deaths in infancy: CESDI SUDI study) is the first to be conducted after the fall in the rate of cot death in the United Kingdom and was specifically designed to identify whether known risk factors had changed or new factors had emerged.

We investigated the risks associated with different sleeping environments and how factors relating to parenting practice, both routine and specific, affect the infant.

Methods

The methodological detail of the study and socio-demographic details have been fully described elsewhere[18] and the results from the first two years published.[19] Briefly, it was a large population based case-control study initially conducted in three former health regions (South West, Northern, and Yorkshire) for two years (February 1993 to January 1995) and expanded (Wessex and Northern regions) for a third year (April 1995 to March 1996). The study aimed to include all cases of sudden unexpected deaths of infants aged 7 to 364 days from a total study population of 17.7 million. Data were collected on a questionnaire by research interviewers and from medical records. Bereaved families were visited within days of the death for a narrative account and again within two weeks to complete the questionnaire. Four controls for each

case were selected. The health visitor for the infant who died was asked to identify two babies on her case list born in the two weeks before the index baby and two babies born in the two weeks after the index baby. In the few instances when the family identified was not available or declined to be interviewed or when the health visitor thought inclusion inappropriate -- for example, because of recent bereavement -- then the family with the baby next closest in age was substituted. The interviewer visited each control family within a week of the index death to collect the same data as for the index case. A period of sleep (the "reference sleep") was identified in the control infant's life in the 24 hours before the interview corresponding to the time of day during which the index baby had died. The questionnaire included a total of over 600 fields, including demographic and social data; the medical history of the infant and other family members; use of cigarettes, alcohol, and drugs; the precise sleeping arrangements for the infant; and full details of the events preceding and the circumstances surrounding the death (or relevant sleep).

Cause of death was established by a multidisciplinary committee after a full paediatric necropsy to a standard protocol was performed. All deaths were classified according to the Avon clinicopathological system.[18]

Statistical methods

Data that were not normally distributed were described by using medians and interquartile ranges. Odds ratios, 95% confidence intervals, and P values for the univariate and multivariate analysis were calculated, taking into account matching with conditional logistic regression by using the statistical package SAS.[20] The age of the control infant was taken as the age at the reference sleep in the 24 hours before the interview. Because of the time lag to arrange four control interviews the control infants were on average about 10 days older than the index infants. The variable for infant age was therefore included in all univariate and multivariate analyses. Models were constructed with the backward stepwise procedure for variables significant at the 5% level in the univariate analysis. When the data were split for analysis (for example, younger and older infants) the four controls were partitioned, regardless of age, into the same subgroup as the corresponding index infant.

Results

Ascertainment

In the three year period there were 456 sudden unexpected deaths in infancy, of which 363 were classified as the sudden infant death syndrome[18] Of these 363 families, 24 refused an interview and 14 were excluded from the analysis because of police involvement (suspected non-accidental injury), because they lived outside the study regions, or because they could not be traced. Each excluded control family (7.9%) was immediately replaced, yielding 325 cases and 1300 controls.

Where the infant slept: usual night time practice

For all families the most common night time sleeping arrangement was that the infant slept in a cot in the parents' bedroom. Table 1 examines room sharing, solitary sleeping, and bed sharing in relation to social class. Infants who usually shared the parents' bed were at increased risk in this univariate analysis, regardless of the socioeconomic breakdown. The figure shows the same data, dividing infants into three age bands. The difference in the prevalence of bed sharing between infants who died and the control infants was greatest for those aged less than 60 days.

Table 1 Infant's sleeping environment(*) in relation to parents: usual night time practice and socioeconomic breakdown. Figures are numbers (percentage) of babies

(*) Usual room sharer: infants who usually shared parental bedroom but not bed; usual solitary sleeper: infants who usually slept in room separate from parents either alone or with other siblings; usual bed sharer: infants who usually shared parental bed for more than two nights/week.

([dagger]) Adjusted for infant age.

([double dagger]) Socioeconomic classes based on highest occupational classification (I being highest, V being lowest) of parents and take previous occupation into account (thus unemployed classification comprises households that have never received waged income).

([paragraph]) For five infants who died and one control infant insufficient information on sleeping environment was collected to include them in analysis.

([paragraph]) For two infants who died and three control infants socioeconomic class of family could not be accurately ascertained.

Where the infant slept: last or reference sleep

Table 2 shows the infants' sleeping environment for the last or reference sleep. A greater proportion of index infants slept in the same bed or in a separate room from their parents. This breakdown is different from the usual night time practice (table 1), partly because of the broad definition of bed sharing and partly because some of the deaths and matched reference sleeps occurred during the day (54 (16.9%) deaths and 205 (15.8%) controls), when the routine was different. Restriction of the analysis to night time deaths yielded virtually identical results to those detailed here.

Table 2 Infant's sleeping environment in relation to parents: last or reference sleep. Figures are numbers (percentage) of babies

(*) For four infants who died and one control infant insufficient information on sleeping environment was collected to include them in analysis,

([dagger]) Adjusted for infant age.

([double dagger]) Includes all infants who shared same sleeping place (bed or sofa) with at least one parent for any part of last or reference sleep.

Table 3 shows a multicategorical variable that differentiates bed sharers into those infants who shared the bed but were put back in their own cot, those infants who shared the bed either for the whole sleep or were found in the parental bed at the end of the sleep, and those infants who slept on a sofa with a parent. The multivariate model is based on all factors significant in the univariate analysis.

Table 3 Infant's sleeping environment in relation to parents: last or reference sleep. Figures are numbers (percentage) of babies

(*) Adjusted for infant age.

([dagger]) Controlled for maternal age (continuous variable); parity (including index or control: 1 child v 2, 3 or 4, 5 or more); gestational age ([is greater than or equal to] 39 weeks v 37-38 weeks, <37 weeks); birth weight (continuous variable); multiple births (singleton v twin or triplets); unemployment (at least one parent employed v both or single mother unemployed); overcrowding (number of people per room excluding toilet, bathroom, hallway, and kitchen if not dining room (<2 v [is greater than or equal to] 2); maternal smoking during pregnancy (no v yes); paternal smoking (no v yes); paternal drug use (since baby was born: never or only once taken illegal substance v two or more times); daily postnatal exposure to tobacco smoke (parental estimate of hours infant exposed per day: 0 v 1 or more hours); previous episode of apparent life threatening event according to parents (none v 1 or more); maternal anxiety over infant becoming too hot (not anxious v anxious); infant put down in prone or side position for last sleep (supine v prone, side); infant being found after last sleep with bedcovers over head (no v yes); use of dummy for any part of last sleep (no v yes); use of pillow (no v yes); recent maternal alcohol consumption before last sleep ([is less than or equal to] 2 units v >2 units); parental estimate of poor health (good or fair v poor); parental tiredness (longest period of infant sleep in previous 24 hours before last sleep (>4 v [is less than or equal to] 4 hours); change in routine affecting infant (not at home, having visitors, non-parental carer, etc: no v yes); sleeping under duvet and thickness (0 tog v 1-4 tog, 5-8 tog, [is less than or equal to] 9 tog). All of these factors were significant in multivariate model with exception of last variable, which was just above 5% significance.

([double dagger]) 12 infants who died and 39 control infants then put back in own room, rest put back in parental bedroom.

([sections]) 54/82 infants who died shared bed for whole sleep compared with 74/189 control infants.

([paragraph]) Two control infants did not share sofa by end of sleep.

There was no increased risk of death for infants who shared the bed but were put back in their own cot but a significant association with those infants who bed shared for the whole sleep or were taken to and found in the parental bed, infants who slept in a separate room from their parents, and infants who shared a sofa. The strengths of these associations increased dramatically in the multivariate model. Some factors in the multivariate model predominantly involved infants sleeping in a cot rather than the parental bed, such as infants being put down in the prone sleeping position (20.8% deaths in a cot v 2.5% deaths in a shared bed), placed on a pillow (11.6% v 1.2%), or infants being found with their heads covered (19.0% v 6.9%). Removal of these three variables halved the strength of the association with being found in a shared bed (multivariate odds ratio 4.62; 95% confidence interval 2.34 to 9.09). The proportion of index infants found in the prone position was three times higher among infants found in a cot compared with the parental bed (4&4% v 14.1(/0).

Of those infants found in the parental bed for the last or reference sleep, some parents brought their infants to bed for breast feeding (14.8% v 21.2% controls) or bottle feeding (14.8% v 7.4%); more of the index parents did so because they usually slept that way (44.4% v 31.2%) and more of the control parents because the baby would not settle (19.8% v 30.7%). Few parents had brought the infant into bed because the baby was unwell (1.2% v 3.7%), because the baby was cold (1.2% v 1.6%), or for a cuddle and extra time in bed (3.7% v 4.2%).

A similar proportion of these infants slept with just one parent in the bed (18.9(/0 v 17.3%), almost always the mother. Most infants slept next to one parent (73.1% v 63.0%), but more control infants slept between parents (22.0% v 37.0%). Two index infants were found under a parent, one was found at the bottom of the bed, and one was found on the floor.

The infants who died in the parental bed (median (interquartile range) age 8 (4-13) weeks) were much younger than those who were found elsewhere (15 (10-23) weeks). When the data were split into younger and older infants by using the median age of all study infants (14 weeks 2 days), the risk associated with bed sharing for older infants found in the parental bed (odds ratio 1.08; 95% confidence interval 0.55 to 2.11) was almost unity but for the younger infants the associated risk was significant(4.65; 2.70 to 7.99).

The index infants who shared a sofa with a parent (median age 9 weeks; 6 to 15 weeks) were similar in age to those who died in the parental bed. Sofa sharing was as common with mothers as fathers. The narrative account suggests that for four deaths the infant was wedged between the parent and the back of the sofa, while for five the parent and infant lay at opposite ends, although we do not have data on the size of the sofa or the position of the adult's legs. Seven of the index parents had not intended to fall asleep on the sofa, but for nine this practice was not unusual. In five of these deaths the cosleeping parent had consumed up to 2 units of alcohol, and three had consumed much more than this.

Associated risks

Table 4 examines the relation between parental smoking and infants found bed sharing. The proportion of index infants found dead in bed with parents who did not smoke was much lower than for control families who did not smoke (2.2% v 7.9% controls). The high prevalence of smoking among index parents (84.2%) was even higher among those parents whose infant was found in the parental bed at the end of the last or reference sleep (91.4%). Among index mothers who smoked, more of those whose infants shared the bed smoked more than 20 cigarettes a day (23.2% v 1.5% controls) compared with those who did not bed share (16.6% v 5.9%).

Table 4 Infants found bed sharing at end of sleep and parental smoking. Figures are numbers (percentage) of infants unless stated otherwise

(*) Mother or partner at time of interview.

([dagger]) Adjusted for infant age.

The multivariate analysis suggests that there are some variables for which the risk is exclusively associated with the cot environment yet increase the significance of the risk associated with the bed sharing environment. Table 5 shows the more restricted model of factors that were predominantly associated with being found in the parental bed after the last or reference sleep. These included recent maternal alcohol consumption of more than 2 units (21.0% deaths in a shared bed v 8.6% deaths in a cot), infants covered by a duvet (61.0% v 31.9%), parental tiredness (54.6% v 20.3%), and sleeping environments in overcrowded conditions (8.5% v 2.5%). Individually each of these variables reduced the significance of the risk associated with bed sharing, and when all were put into a collective model, being found in a shared bed was no longer significant.

Table 5 Multivariate model controlling for adverse bed sharing conditions. Figures are numbers (percentage) of infants unless stated otherwise

(*) Reference group for sleeping environment (first four variables) was infants who shared room, reference group for duvet thickness (last three variables) was infants who did not use duvet.

([dagger]) Defined as longest period of infant sleep in previous 24 hours before last sleep: >4 v [is less than or equal to] 4 hours).

([double dagger]) 2 units of alcohol in 24 hours before last or reference sleep.

([sections]) Defined as >2 people per room (excluding kitchen not used for dining, toilet, bathroom, and hallways).

Discussion

Study findings

Our results show that infants sharing a sofa with an adult during sleep is associated with a particularly high and previously unrecognised risk of the sudden infant death syndrome. The apparent risk associated with infants sleeping in a separate room from the parents and sharing a bed for the whole or last part of sleep is less clear because of potential confounding. We found no increase in risk for older infants who share the parents' bed, infants of parents who do not smoke, or when the infant is returned to his or her cot. In the more restricted model, which incorporated factors for which there was an a priori expectation of their potential effects on the sleep environment, the independent significance of sharing the bed at the end of sleep disappeared. This result suggests that the risk of death may be associated with other potentially modifiable adverse conditions rather than the practice of bed sharing itself, although interpretation should be cautious given the prior expectation of this particular analysis.

The increased relative risk of death among infants who sleep in a separate room, regardless of infant age or risks associated with the cot environment, is consistent with the findings of the New Zealand study,[21] although for a third of both cases and controls, this was a day time practice and not necessarily the night time routine. Cosleeping in terms of room sharing increases the sensory exchanges possible between parent and infants,[22] but further research is required to investigate whether room sharing is protective in itself or merely a marker for hidden confounders not measured in this study.

Problems with interpretation

Our results highlight some of the methodological difficulties inherent in multivariate modelling. In the large multivariate model the infant who was found sharing the bed at the end of the sleep seemed to be an important risk factor. Within this model, however, it was difficult to quantify the strength of the risk given that the interaction of certain cofactors was based on the environment in which the infant slept; nor is it possible to generalise these multivariate findings to the whole population. Certain factors characteristic of infants found in the parental bed were systematically different from those where the infant was found in a cot: bed sharing infants were much younger, few were put down in the prone position, and few were found with their heads covered. Conversely these factors were reversed among infants who slept separately from their parents.

The high prevalence of smokers among the bed sharing parents of infants who died confirms the findings of Mitchell[21] and makes it impossible to derive precise estimates of the risk associated with bed sharing for families who do not smoke.

Informed evidence

The practices of sharing the bed and cosleeping are culturally diverse. For example, a baby sleeping at arm's length from the mother on a firm surface, as is often the case in Hong Kong,[23] or a Pacific Island baby sleeping on the bed rather than in the bed[21] is in a different environment from a baby sleeping in direct contact with the mother on a soft mattress and covered by a thick duvet. Previous observational studies have highlighted contributory factors such as parental alcohol consumption, parental fatigue, a lack of an alternative sleeping place, and the use of thick duvets[10-12]; our data have extended these observations and given quantitative estimates of the relative risks for these factors.

Unlike cots, which are designed to meet safety standards for infants, adult beds are not so designed and may, at least in theory, carry a risk of accidental entrapment and suffocation. This and the conflicting evidence for harm or benefit from bed sharing shown in previous studies have generated strong professional beliefs about the appropriateness or otherwise of sharing a bed and cosleeping.

The debate on the safety, advantages, and disadvantages of bed sharing and cosleeping must be informed by evidence from epidemiology, physiology, and anthropology if it is to become more than the exchange of mere opinion. There has been little in the way of direct observational data until recently, but it is becoming clear that sharing a bed both for infants and mothers results in complex interactions that are completely different from isolated sleeping and that need to be understood in detail before application of simplistic labels such as "safe" or "unsafe."[24 25] Our results suggest that, as McKenna has argued, perhaps it is not bed sharing per se that is hazardous but rather the particular circumstances in which bed sharing occurs. That some of these circumstances may be modifiable has important implications in terms of social policy and health education.

Key messages

* Cosleeping with an infant on a sofa was associated with a particularly high risk of sudden infant death syndrome

* Sharing a room with the parents was associated with a lower risk

* There was no increased risk associated with bed sharing when the infant was placed back in his or her cot

* Among parents who do not smoke or infants older than 14 weeks there was no association between infants being found in the parental bed and an increased risk of sudden infant death syndrome

* The risk linked with bed sharing among younger infants seems to be associated with recent parental consumption of alcohol, overcrowded housing conditions, extreme parental tiredness, and the infant being under a duvet

Contributors: PSB, PJF, IJS, MWP, PJB, and JG were all involved in the concepts and design from the beginning of the study. The analysis was conducted by PSB under the supervision of the others, and all were involved in the interpretations of the data, drafting, and revisions. PSB, PJF, and IJS supervised data collection for the whole study period within their particular regions. MWP supervised data collection for the final year of the study. PJB supervised collection of postmortem samples and data from all the regions. JG provided epidemiological advice. JY, a research nurse who runs the sleep laboratory in Bristol, and Pam Nadin, a research health visitor, were involved in the interpretation of the data and subsequent analyses, drafting, and revisions of this paper. PJF is the guarantor for the study.

Funding: The National Advisory Body for CESDI and the Foundation for the Study of Infant Deaths.

Competing interests: None declared.

[1] Mosko S, McKenna J, Dickel M, Hunt L. Parent-infant co-sleeping: the appropriate context for the study of infant sleep and implications for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) research. J Behav Med 1993;16:589-610.

[2] Anderson GC. Current knowledge about skin-to-skin (kangaroo) care for preterm infants. J Perinatol 1991; 11:216-26.

[3] Ludington-Hoe SM, Hadeed AJ, Anderson GC. Physiological responses to skin-to-skin contact in hospitalised premature infants. J Perinatol 1991;11:19-24.

[4] Luke JL. Sleeping arrangements of sudden infant death syndrome victims in the District of Columbia--a preliminary report. J Forensic Sci 1978;23:379-83.

[5] Klonoff-Cohen HS, Edelstein SL. Bed sharing and the sudden infant death syndrome. BMJ 1995;311:1269-72.

[6] Lee NNY, Chan YF, Davis DP, Lau E, Yip DCP. Sudden infant death syndrome in Hong Kong: confirmation of a low incidence. BMJ 1989;298:721-2.

[7] Carpenter RG. Sudden and unexpected deaths in infancy (cot death). In: Camps FE, Carpenter RG, eds. Sudden and unexpected deaths in infancy (cot death). Bristol: John Wright, 1972:7-15.

[8] Bass M, Kravath RE, Glass LG. Death-scene investigation in sudden infant death. N Engl J Med 1986;315:100-5.

[9] Rintahaka PJ, Hirvonen J. The epidemiology of sudden infant death syndrome in Finland in 1969-80. Forens Sci Int 1986;30:219-33.

[10] Mitchell EA, Taylor BJ, Ford RPK, Stewart AW, Becroft DM, Thompson JM, et al. Four modifiable and other major risk factors for cot death: the New Zealand study. NJ Paediatr Child Health 1992;28:1:3-S8.

[11] Mitchell EA, Stewart AW, Scragg R, Ford RPK, Taylor BJ, Becroft DMO, et al. Ethnic differences in mortality from sudden infant death syndrome in New Zealand. BMJ 1993;306:13-6.

[12] Hoffman HJ, Hunter JC, Ellish NJ, Janerich DT, Goldberg J. Adverse reproductive factors and the sudden infant death syndrome. In: Harper RM, Hoffman HJ, eds. Sudden infant death syndrome. Risk factors and basic mechanisms. New York: PMA Publishing, 1988:153-75.

[13] Takeda KA. A possible mechanism of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS).J Kyoto Prefecture University Med 1987;96:965-8.

[14] Davies DP. Cot death in Hong Kong. A rare problem? Lancet 1985;ii:1346-7.

[15] Gantley M, Davies DP, Murcott A. Sudden infant death syndrome. Links with infant care practices. BMJ 1993;16:263-22.

[16] Farooqi S, Lip GYH, Beevers DG. Bed sharing and smoking in sudden infant death syndrome. BMJ 1994;308:204-5.

[17] Tuohy PG, Counsell AM, Geddis DC. Sociodemographic factors associated with sleeping position and location. Arch Dis Child 1993;69:664-6.

[18] Leach CEA, Blair PS, Fleming PJ, Smith IJ, Ward Platt M, Berry PJ, et al. Sudden unexpected deaths in infancy: similarities and differences in the epidemiology of SIDS and explained deaths. Pediatrics (in press).

[19] Fleming PJ, Blair PS, Bacon C, Bensley D, Smith I, Taylor E, et al. Environment of infants during sleep and risk of sudden infant death syndrome: results of 1993-5 case-control study for confidential inquiry into stillbirths and deaths in infancy. BMJ 1996 ;313:191-5.

[20] SAS Institute. SAS technical report P-229, SAS/STAT software: changes and enhancements, release 6.07. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, 1992.

[21] Mitchell EA, Thompson JMD. Co-sleeping increases the risk of SIDS, but sleeping in the parents' bedroom lowers it. In: TO Rognum, ed. Sudden infant death syndrome. New trends in the nineties. Oslo: Scandanavian University Press, 1995.

[22] McKenna J, Mosko S, Richard C, Drummond S, Hunt L, Cetel MB, et al. Experimental studies of infant-parent co-sleeping: mutual physiological and behavioural influences and their relevance to SIDS (sudden infant death syndrome). Early Hum Dev 1994;38:187-201.

[23] Nelson EAS, Chan PH. Child care practices and cot death in Hong Kong. N Z Med J 1996;109:144-6.

[24] McKenna. Sudden infant death syndrome in cross-cultural perspective. Is infant-parent cosleeping protective? Ann Rev Anthropol 1996;25:201-16.

[25] Ball HL, Hooker E, Kelly PJ. Where will the babies sleep? Attitudes and practices of new and experienced parents regarding co-sleeping with their new- born infant. Am Anthropol (in press).

(Accepted 24 August 1999)

Institute of Child Health, Royal Hospital for Children, St Michael's Hill, Bristol BS2 8BJ

Peter Blair medical statistician

Peter Fleming professor of infant health and developmental physiology

Jeanine Young research nurse

Pam Nadin research health visitor

P J Berry professor of paediatric pathology

Jean Golding professor of paediatric and perinatal epidemiology

Nuffield Institute for Health Services, Leeds LS2 9PL

Iain Smith senior lecturer in health research and honorary consultant in child health

Newcastle Neonatal Service, Royal Victoria Infirmary, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 4LP

Martin Ward Platt consultant paediatrician and senior lecturer in child health

Correspondence to: P Blair p.s.blair@bris.ac.uk

BMJ 1999;319:1457-62

COPYRIGHT 1999 British Medical Association
COPYRIGHT 2000 Gale Group

Return to Sudden infant death syndrome
Home Contact Resources Exchange Links ebay